Jump to content

Article - navigation


Guest Pigeon_Man87
 Share

Recommended Posts

Navigation and Homing in Pigeons:  A Brief Sketch  Gordon A Chalmers, DVMLethbridge, Alberta, CanadaEmail:  gacdvm@telus.net *****

Navigation and homing in pigeons and migratory birds continue to be fascinating yet puzzling subjects.  Although many strides have been made over a number of years in understanding some of the complexities associated with this subject, much remains a mystery to this day.  Even so, modern research continues to peel away the layers – albeit slowly - surrounding this mystery.  The following information discusses some of the currently available knowledge on the multifaceted subject of navigation in pigeons and other birds.

 

There seems to be general support for the idea that pigeons use a two-step method to home accurately:  firstly, a map to determine their home direction and secondly, a compass to guide them.          It is known that presence of the sun (sun compass) allows pigeons to navigate correctly.  It seems that they are able to compensate for the movement of the sun across the sky and hence, to stay on course.  However, finding the home direction doesn’t require a view of the sun.  In order to navigate in the absence of sun on cloudy days, the birds appear to use cues from the lines of magnetic force that run from pole to pole over the entire earth.  The belief is that pigeons, like other birds, have an inborn magnetic sense that they use as a directional compass.            The liquid core of the earth produces a magnetic field that is a dipole (di = two) field in which the magnetic poles lie near the geographic north and south poles.  The field lines of the magnetic field leave the ground at the Antarctic pole, curve around the earth and then re-enter the earth at the Arctic pole.  Magnetic north and geographic north differ by a certain number of degrees.  This variation or deviation may be considerable near the magnetic poles but it soon becomes much less at lower latitudes.  In most parts of the world the field lines run roughly north-south.  The total intensity of the field decreases gradually from very high values at the poles to about half that strength near the magnetic equator.  Superimposed on this general pattern are certain irregularities.  For instance different degrees of magnetization of rocks cause localized magnetic anomalies; as well, time variations of the geomagnetic field occur on a daily basis.  These regular daily variations plus the irregular fluctuations that occur because of magnetic storms from space, etc., are important in consideration of a navigational map used by birds.  Because of its structure, the magnetic field is present at all times, and therefore, is a constant, reliable source of information.  The magnetic field seems to provide directional information for birds, and factors such as its total intensity and inclination may provide birds with information about their geographical position. The use of magnetic information in the orientation of birds was discussed as early as the 1800s.  As long ago as 1859, one German researcher proposed what we now call a magnetic compass in birds. The magnetic compass was described first in the European robin, a small bird that migrates at night, and experimental work showed that these birds use the magnetic field in direction finding.  The magnetic compass of birds is what is known as an inclination compass – instead of indicating north and south it distinguishes between poleward and equatorward.A number of other species of birds, including short and long-distance migrants from different countries over the world, have been found to use a magnetic compass; most members of this group are night migrants.  Studies in pigeons have suggested that their magnetic compass too, is an inclination compass.  This information indicates that the magnetic inclination compass is a very widespread mechanism among birds in general regardless of their relationship, their geographic distribution and their migratory habits.  It also indicates that migratory direction is genetically encoded in birds as a compass course relative to the geomagnetic field.  In both north and south hemispheres, birds begin their Autumn migration heading toward the equator – south for northern birds, north for southern birds.  During homing, the magnetic compass is involved not only in locating the course determined by navigational methods.  It seems that it may also be a part of these navigational processes themselves, that is, when these processes are based on information obtained during the trip from the loft to the release point.  One experiment showed that young, inexperienced pigeons were disoriented after they were released when they had travelled to the release point in a planned distortion of the magnetic field.  However, a second group that had experienced the same distorted magnetic field for the same length of time, this time while they waited at the release site, oriented correctly and headed for home.  The authors of this study concluded that it wasn’t the distorted magnetic field as such but being transported in a distorted field that was critical.  On this basis it has been assumed that pigeons use the magnetic field as a reference system for storing information about the course of the outward trip as well as the homeward course, and that the distortion of the magnetic field during the trip to the release point prevented the first group of birds from storing such information.  Only very young inexperienced birds apparently use this strategy; older more experienced birds don’t appear to be affected by magnetic distortions during the trip to the release point – which suggests a change in their navigational strategy that may be related to their greater experience. The magnetic compass is involved in the learning curve that leads to the establishment of the sun compass.  Young inexperienced pigeons use the magnetic compass before they can use the sun compass.  There is evidence that the magnetic compass serves as a directional reference system to establish the sun compass.  Later on, the sun compass becomes the preferred system.  When pigeons are used to flying in overcast conditions, their orientation toward home under either sunny or overcast skies is similar, so it would appear that the accuracy of the magnetic compass is equal to that of the sun compass.  How accurate is the magnetic compass?  The flight behaviour of birds suggests considerable accuracy.  When pigeons are released in cloudy conditions in which they have to depend on their magnetic compass, they may produce compass directions of high accuracy, deviating from a direct line by less than 25o.  Research has also shown that there is a pronounced learning stage involving the navigational map during training in the first 2-3 months of life after weaning. There is marked development in the first to second month, followed by a critical period between the second and third months of age, during which development is impaired possibly as a result of a reorganization of the system, and then slight progress occurs in subsequent months.  A related study in 2005 concluded that lofts apparently play a central function in the development of the navigational system, but their exact role is unknown. It is known that a wide group of bacteria and algae orient their movements along lines of the magnetic field.  The basis for this ability was determined to be crystals of the magnetic, iron-based minerals magnetite, also known as lodestone (Fe3O4 -- ferric oxide), and greigite (Fe3S4 – ferric sulphide).  Later, magnetite was also found in honeybees, birds, salmon, sea turtles and a number of other animals known to orient with the magnetic field of the earth.  Most magnetite isolated from animals has been in the form of what are called single-domain crystals that are particles of magnetite that are attracted by a magnet but do not attract iron particles to themselves.  In fact, they are extremely tiny permanently magnetized magnets that twist into alignment with the magnetic field.  In some animals such as pigeons,  however, magnetite crystals are smaller than single-domain size; these smaller crystals are said to be super para-magnetic and have different magnetic properties.  Unlike single-domain crystals, they don’t have a permanent magnetic movement and so they can’t physically rotate into alignment with the magnetic field of the earth. Even though these crystals don’t move, their magnetic alignment tracks the alignment of the surrounding magnetic field.  In order for magnetite crystals to function as magnetic receptors, the assumption is that they must have to make contact with the nervous system.  The strongest evidence that this is so is derived from experiments with trout and pigeons.  In pigeons and other birds, crystals that are thought to be magnetite have been found in the upper beak.  Electron microscopic studies (that magnify many thousands of times) of this area in pigeons have shown that these crystals are located within the ends of nerves and are distributed along the membrane of nerve cells.  In pigeons, these crystals have been found to be super para-magnetic.  They are present in clusters, and in pigeons about 10-15 of these clusters are found inside the end of one nerve.  An interesting similarity between birds and fish is that the location of the crystals in the beak of pigeons and in the nasal area of trout, appears to receive its nerve supply from the ophthalmic (eye) branch of the trigeminal nerve, one of the 10 nerves that originate directly from the under side of the brain.  The implication is that the ophthalmic branch of the trigeminal nerve carries to the brain, important information about the magnetic field.  (As an aside, these findings indicate that pigeons and other birds readily obtain the extremely tiny amounts of magnetite they need from their regular diet, mineral mix, water, etc., so I have long wondered why fanciers would buy magnetite!)

 

As an aside, it is interesting that Carey Oler, a scientist from my area, is the son of one of my club mates, and an expert in geomagnetic disturbances.  He makes the point that races from east or west are more difficult for pigeons to navigate, since the birds are crossing magnetic field lines.  Birds racing from north or south fare better because they are flying along the field lines of the magnetic field.  In a recent ARPJ article, Bill Verco touched on a similar point about north vs east courses and their effects on racing. Adding to the complexity of navigation and homing was the interesting suggestion by some researchers that the magnetic compass of birds might require light for its function. This idea was put to the test by the German research team of Wolfgang and Roswitha Wiltschko who found that European robins that were exposed to the local geomagnetic field oriented correctly in the expected migratory direction under blue, turquoise and green light (short wavelength light) but were disoriented under yellow and red light (long wavelength light).  Thus, the orientational responses of birds to magnetic cues seem to depend on the wavelength of light.  (When ‘white’ light from the sun is passed through a prism, it separates in this order into its seven colors -- red, orange, yellow, green, blue, indigo and violet -- each of which has a different wavelength.  It is interesting that many years earlier, the first attempt by the same authors to establish the role of light in magnetic orientation involved experimental studies with young, inexperienced pigeons.  As noted previously, to determine the correct homeward direction, inexperienced young pigeons rely on compass information that they obtain during the trip from the loft to the release point.  If these inexperienced pigeons are transported in the normal geomagnetic field and in darkness as well, they become disoriented when they are released.  (However, experienced older pigeons aren’t affected by similar treatment.)  These findings in young pigeons are consistent with their use of a magnetic compass mechanism that also depends on light for correct orientation to the home loft.  Experiments with young homing pigeons suggest a similar relationship between the wavelength of light and the ability to obtain directional information from the magnetic field.  In summary, the available data indicate that light from the blue-green part of the visual spectrum is required for magnetic reception in birds.To add to this discovery, the same researchers found that that the effect of light on the magnetic compass in European robins was processed through only one eye:  the right one.  When a cap was placed over the left eye, these birds continued to orient correctly, but when the right eye was covered, the birds became disoriented.  Similarly, work with pigeons showed that birds with the left eye covered homed consistently better than those with the right eye covered.  Because the left side of the brain controls the right side of the body - and vice versa - these findings suggested the importance of the right eye and left side of the brain in processes involving flight control, navigation and homing in pigeons. Adding further to the navigational mix is the controversial subject of odours and their role in the navigation of pigeons.  In 1971 an Italian scientist named Papi and his colleagues proposed the idea that odours played an important role in navigation and homing by pigeons.  In the years since then the issue has remained contentious and has resulted in many experiments, theories, proponents and of course, dissent.  The basic premise behind this idea is that pigeons learn an odour ‘map’ by associating odours detected at the home loft with the direction from which they are carried on the wind, or by sensing odours during exercise around the loft and surrounding territory.  Some of the evidence in favor of this idea:  1) birds whose sense of smell has been blocked by anesthetic or surgery don’t orient or home from unfamiliar release sites; 2) birds that are shipped long distances don’t orient unless they can sample the natural air during the trip; 3) if the air supplied to pigeons is filtered through charcoal, their ability to orient is eliminated.  However, it’s not obvious how different strengths of odours that might be useful over ranges of hundreds of miles could be maintained in the dynamic changing atmosphere, and some scientists use this point to reject the whole idea of odours and navigation.  Without knowing the identity of air-borne substances that constitute the foundation for a map based on odours, there is no way to know much about their distribution in space and over time.  Even so, some scientists offer the conclusion that the accumulated evidence supports the idea that the sense of smell plays a role in the map component of homing.  Not everyone agrees and the debate carries on.  The location of the loft seems to make an important difference in the orientation of pigeons.  In one study in Germany, pigeons were housed in two different lofts, one in a garden location sheltered from the wind, and the other located four stories higher with free access to the wind.  Pigeons in both lofts then had their sense of smell blocked (either by surgery or anesthetic).  The birds in the sheltered loft oriented and homed normally, whereas those in the loft located four stories high, neither oriented nor homed.  The researchers explained that the difference between the two groups of birds was likely related to their access to the wind and the odours it carried.  The garden loft was sheltered from the wind; pigeons reared in this loft may have learned to rely on cues that were not related to odours in the wind, and as a result they oriented and homed correctly.  The other group in the four-storey loft where there was free access to the wind and the odours it brought, may have relied extensively on orientation/homing cues detected in these odours.  Because of their reliance on wind-borne odours, this group was unable to orient or home when their sense of smell was blocked. On the basis of cumulative research, it seems evident that pigeons use many different cues, possibly including odours, to develop a map; how and why these multiple cues are selected at any given time is unknown. Some other interesting information - in recent years, at least two similar research programs, one a 10-year study from Oxford University in the UK, and the other from a team of European researchers, have come to the same conclusion:  that pigeons often home along major highways and use landmarks to remember where they are.  In the European study, investigators used the Global Positioning System (GPS) to record 216 flight paths of pigeons over distances up to 30 miles.  Information from this GPS study showed that experienced pigeons released from familiar sites over three years around Rome, were significantly attracted to highways and a railway track running toward home, and not to the direct bee-line toward home.  Birds often broke away from highways that veered away from home but many birds continued along these highways to a major junction even when the detour added appreciably to the flight.  Significant road following occurred in 40-50% of flight paths along one particular line of flight, from northwest to southeast.  The authors suggested that pigeons use a learned route-following strategy and the use of particular geographic locations – landmarks in other words -- for final navigation to the loft.  

 

Because of the open prairie country in which I live, during training on the east course, I have often been able to follow my own birds along a main highway and a nearby parallel-running railway track.  Usually I see these birds flying at treetop level within 10 to 50 yards of these structures.  Very commonly they will deviate slightly to pass over small towns, farms, etc. that are adjacent to the highway-railway.  It’s very much a heart-stopping experience to see them racing into a headwind as they fly two to three feet directly above the highway, and often lifting up just barely in time to avoid traffic approaching from the front and rear!

 

The ability to detect ultraviolet light and to hear infrasound (frequency less than 20 Hz) has been demonstrated in pigeons.  (Note - Hz [hertz] is the unit of frequency; one hertz has a periodic interval of one second; kHz- one thousand periods per second.  The range of human hearing is from 0 Hz to roughly 20 kHz.)  The possible negative effects of infrasound on several races held in Europe and the USA, based on an article by Dr JT Hagstrum, were discussed in an earlier article in the ARPJ. In summary, as researchers have long suspected, there are likely many factors involved in the complexity of avian navigation.  Over all, the general role of magnetic cues in the navigational map used by pigeons is still not clear.  Homing is hardly affected by manipulations of the magnetic field, which suggests that magnetic cues are not essential for use of the map.  It seems that pigeons and other birds have several alternative cues from which they can select a number of possibilities to aid in navigation.  However, the conditions that determine the favoured cues used at any given time remain a mystery.  Research into this complex, fascinating subject continues unabated!*****

 

 

 

 

 

 

Link to comment
Share on other sites

  • 1 month later...

Fascinating article......... and perhaps explains why this is the most difficult place I've lived in for holding on to birds. We fly east  to west and as I've said before there have been several fanciers who moved to the south west (Devon & Cornwall) who have brought birds with them that made them club champions where they came from. However the birds did nothing and quickly dissapeared down here (to be replaced by birds that moved them back to the top ), there is no doubt Percy is a big reason.......... but this articles theory would be susbtantiated by what has happened to such fancier's.

Link to comment
Share on other sites

Join the conversation

You can post now and register later. If you have an account, sign in now to post with your account.

Guest
Reply to this topic...

×   Pasted as rich text.   Paste as plain text instead

  Only 75 emoji are allowed.

×   Your link has been automatically embedded.   Display as a link instead

×   Your previous content has been restored.   Clear editor

×   You cannot paste images directly. Upload or insert images from URL.

Loading...
 Share

  • Advert: Morray Firth One Loft Classic
  • Advert: M.A.C. Lofts Pigeon Products
  • Advert: RV Woodcraft
  • Advert: B.Leefe & Sons
  • Advert: Apex Garden Buildings
  • Advert: Racing Pigeon Supplies
  • Advert: Solway Feeders


×
×
  • Create New...